Unless you are still in the flower of your youth, you
probably think that college students are weird.
Now, psychologists have decided that the word should also be
an acronym, standing, as Bethany Brookshire explains for: “Western, educated, and from industrialized,
rich, and democratic countries.”
The acronym was necessitated by the fact that a goodly
amount of psychological research, the kind that is conducted in college
laboratories, limits its sampling to a population of people that is basically
WEIRD.
But does this WEIRD group really represent humanity? That,
Brookshire says, is the question:
WEIRD
subjects (perhaps you were one?) are still human, of course, so you might think
that what’s generalizable to them must be generalizable to the rest of humanity.
Graciously, Brookshire accepts that WEIRD college students
are human beings, but that does not make them typically human. While there are
certainly enough similarities between WEIRD college students and human beings,
there are also some significant differences.
The differences are both natural and cultural. Since the
adolescent brain is not fully developed, the results obtained from studying college
students will have a limited application to adults.
And then, there’s the influence of manners, mores and the
culture. No one believes that culture is everything, but culture does count
for something.
Brookshire offers some examples:
WEIRD
subjects, from countries that represent only about 12 percent of the world’s
population, differ from other populations in moral decision making, reasoning style, fairness, even things
like visual perception. This is because a lot of these behaviors and
perceptions are based on the environments and contexts in which we grew up.
There’s a big dose of sociology in our psychology. For example, WEIRD people are better at optical illusions involving line
length, possibly because our environments contain a lot of straight lines
in things like buildings.
We have other reasons to doubt research performed on WEIRD subjects.
Brookshire explains that the group of students is often self-selected; it does
not really care about the study and it is more likely to tell researchers what
they want to hear.
One suspects that many researchers are conducting these studies
in order to cloak their cultural agenda in science.
Among the more bizarre studies is this one, reported by
Brookshire:
How did
you lose your virginity? Maybe it was in a romantic garden under a full moon
with the scent of roses all around, in the arms of your one true love? Maybe it
was at the drunken party after prom night, with your underwear around your
ankles, hoping no one could see you to take pictures? Maybe it was on your
wedding night, maybe it was long before. Maybe it was even long after.
Regardless, I hope you enjoyed it, because a recent
study has shown that your sexual well-being today has a lot to do with
how much you enjoyed it then.
Obviously, it’s an idiot question in the first place, but
the sex life of college students is infinitely fascinating to post grads.
To conduct their research, the scientists set out to produce
a perfectly homogeneous group of subjects:
When
recruiting for many of these WEIRD studies, scientists often make the sample as
homogeneous as possible, in an effort to detect small differences. Take the
virginity study I mentioned above. The researchers eliminated from the sample
anyone whose first exposure to sex involved “physical force” (that is, anyone
who had been raped). This eliminated a small number (12 out of 331
participants). And they eliminated anyone who did not have heterosexual
intercourse. The sample is so homogeneous
that it applies only to heterosexual college students—who on average, according
to information they supplied to the researchers, had lost their virginity only
two years before.
But, if you limit the study to students who resemble each
other, why would the research not be what scientists call anecdotal?
And then, on what scientific grounds can
you predict a lifetime of sexual experience from the accounts of college
students who have had very little sexual experience and who have never been
married?
How did the researchers collect their data? Brookshire
explains:
In the
case of the virginity study, subjects kept an “intimate relations” diary for
two weeks. Out of about 300 participants, the researchers got records of a
total of 639 intimate encounters, about two per person. From these two intimate
encounters per person, in college students who had lost their virginity on
average two years before, the authors concluded that sexual satisfaction in the
present was strongly affected by how you lost your virginity and that the
effects of how you lost your virginity could persist for years to come.
But then, she adds, the results are based on what the
students are willing to divulge and on their own subjective impression of their
own sexual experiences:
… self-report
studies are always open to things like exaggeration or covering up, especially
when it comes to studies about sex, where a lot of cultural pressure comes into
play.
It is not just that these studies are limited to WEIRD
college students. One suspects that it does not even provide an accurate assessment of college student behavior. One starts thinking that these researchers are committing
scientific fraud.
As for the prediction about the future sex life of these students, it is good to recall what Wittgenstein once said: there is no such thing as a fact
about tomorrow.
2 comments:
"that sexual satisfaction in the present was strongly affected by how you lost your virginity and that the effects of how you lost your virginity could persist for years to come."
Why is it so difficult for people to understand the intertwining of *correlation* and *causation*?
It's quite possible, for example, that heavy drinkers tend to lose their virginity in degrading and unsatisfying ways...and that their later sexual experiences are also unsatisfying. Maybe instead of the bad virginity-loss experience causing the later problems, the heavy drinking caused both.
Good point. If they were really scientists, they would understand.
Post a Comment